РефератыИностранный языкDoDolphins Essay Research Paper Bottlenose dolphins are

Dolphins Essay Research Paper Bottlenose dolphins are

Dolphins Essay, Research Paper


Bottlenose dolphins are among the most vocal of the nonhuman animals and


exhibit remarkable development of the sound production and auditory


mechanisms. This can be seen in audition, which is shown in the animal?s


highly refined echolocation ability, and in tightly organized schools in which


they live that are made up by sound communication. In testing the


communication skills of dolphins, extensive studies have been done on vocal


mimicry, in which the animal imitates computer-generated sounds in order to


test motor control in terms of cognitive ability. Language comprehension on


the other hand has been tested through labeling of objects, which has proven


to be successful regarding the association of sound and object stimulus. The


biggest question in dolphin communication, is whether or not the species is


capable of intentional communicative acts. Though results from studies have


been debatable, the key to understanding the extent to this ?language? is to


determine whether they have a repertoire of grammatical rules that generate


organized sequences. In determining this, the greatest accomplishment for


both the scientist and all of humanity, would be to accomplish interspecies


communication, creating a bridge between humans and animals which could


open up a new understanding of the unknown world of wildlife. Most


importantly, it is necessary to understand the incredible aptitude of dolphin


communicative skills, and the impressive intelligence the animal possesses


which allows for a great deal of intraspecies and interspecies communication


(Schusterman, Thomas, & Wood, 1986). The acoustical reception and


processing abilities of the bottlenosed dolphins have generally been shown to


be among the most sophisticated of any animal so far examined (Popper,


1980 as cited by Schusterman et al. 1986). In order to understand the


complexity of these highly mechanized acoustic systems, it is necessary to


learn the process for which the dolphin hears. In most water-adapted


cetaceans, tissue conduction is the primary route of sound conduction to the


middle ear. The isolation of the bullae shows an adaptation for tissue


conducted sound. The lower jaw contains fat that is closely associated with


the impedance of seawater. The lower jawbone of most odontocetes


becomes broadened and quite thin posteriorly, and the fat forms an oval


shape that closely corresponds to the area of minimum thickness of the jaw.


This fat body leads directly to the bulla, producing a sound path to the ear


structures located deep within the head. Paired and single air sacs are


scattered throughout the skull, which serve to channel these tissue-conducted


sounds (Popov & Supin, 1991). Other than this description, there are still


more studies needed to determine the function of the middle ear and the type


of bone conduction that occurs within the bulla. Due to detailed audiograms,


dolphins have been shown to have the ability to detect high-frequency


sounds. In an experiment by Johnson (1966) as cited in Schusterman et al.


(1986), sine-wave sounds ranging in frequency from 75 Hz to 150 Hz were


presented to a bottle-nosed dolphin. The animal was trained to swim in a


stationary area within a stall and to watch for a light to come on. Following


the light presentation a sound was sometimes presented. If the dolphin heard


the sound, its task was to leave the area and push a lever. Sound intensity


levels were varied by a staircase method of 1, 2, or 3 dB steps. The resulting


audiogram, compared to the human aerial audiogram, showed that at regions


of best sensitivity for each, thresholds for human and dolphin are quite similar,


but separated by about 50 kHz in frequency, showing that the animal?s inner


ear function is very similar to a human. The experiments done on dolphin


auditory functions have generally shown a finely adapted sound reception


system. This would be expected due to the highly adapted echolocation


ability of the bottlenosed dolphin and other cetaceans. Results of work on


absolute thresholds, critical bandwidths, frequency discrimination, and sound


localization all indicate that the dolphin auditory system is at least as good or


better than the human system. This is in spite of the fact that sound travels five


times as fast under water as it does in air (Popov et al. 1991). The


bottlenosed dolphin in captivity produces two categories of vocalizations: (a)


narrow-band, frequency-varying, continuous tonal sounds referred to as


?whistles? and (b) broad-band pulsed sounds expressed as trains of very


short duration clicks of varying rates (Evans, 1967, as cited in Schusterman


et al. 1986). The pulsed sounds are used for both communication and


echolocation, and the whistles are found to be used primarily for


communication (Herman & Tavolga, 1980, as cited in Schusterman et al.


1986). Descriptions in literature emphasizing either the whistles or the pulsed


sounds have led to contradictory hypotheses concerning the communication


system of the dolphin. It has been reported that individually specific whistles


often make up over 90% of the whistle repertoire of captive bottlenosed


dolphins (Popov et al. 1991). A number of observations of apparent vocal


mimicry have been made, though with no systematic investigation of the


degree of vocal flexibility. The observed variability in the whistles, combined


with the difficulty of identifying individual vocalizing dolphins in a group, has


led to speculation that the whistles might be a complex, shared system, in


which specific meanings could be assigned to specific whistles. Consideration


of vocal mimicry has been taken to understand its relation to cognitive


complexity, and to the potential use of vocal response for communication in


an artificial language. In one study done by McCowan, Hanser, & Doyle,


(1999), the dolphin was able to learn to mimic a number of


computer-generated model sounds with high fidelity and reliability. The


dolphin using its whistle mode of vocalization imitated all of the sounds, and


all were distinct from the unreinforced whistles produced prior to training.


The large majority of each dolphin?s whistle vocalizations were individually


specific acoustic patterns, described as a ?signature whistle?; the rest of the


whistles were short chirps. The results of the mimicry training have shown that


dolphins can mimic tonal sounds with frequencies between 4 and 20 Hz. Due


to this research, scientists can now learn from these mimicry skills how to


understand and develop natural communication based on a stronger emphasis


on the animal?s cognitive abilities (Brecht, 1993). In object labeling, the


dolphins seemed to understand the task of associating model sounds with


displayed objects. Progress was most rapid when the model sound was


always presented at full intensity, but the probability of its being presented on


any given trial was systematically decreased over successive trials. There


wasn?t any confusion of the objects themselves, but only a tendency to drift in


the quality of the rendition of the labels. This demonstration of symbolic use


of vocalizations could lead to the investigation of the potential of animals to


form referential concepts, thus creating a new understanding of dolphin


communication and its uses in the wild. The main purpose of study in dolphin


language, is the interest in whether the animal?s speech is intentional


communication like our own human speech. The fact that awareness as


applied to the phenomena of human communication also implies something


we would not attribute to animals-and this is the awareness that


communicative acts are behaviors about behaviors (Crook, 1983, as cited in


Schusterman et al. 1986). Language, as we know it, could not exist without


the capacity for intentional communic

ation, as all linguistic communications


are, by definition, intentional. Dolphins have been observed to have some of


these intentional communication characteristics, as their behaviors have


shown in captivity. For example, dolphins have been observed to squirt or


splash water at strangers who come near their tank. After squirting the water


the dolphin will raise itself out of the water to curiously observe what effect


their behavior had on the stranger. Although this behavior is not communitive,


nonetheless, it seems to suggest that the dolphin is aware of the effect of its


behavior on others, showing that it has the cognitive ability for intentional


communication (Erickson, 1993). Communication between humans and


dolphins occurs mostly through a gestural language that borrows some words


from American Sign Language. The trainers make the gestures with big arm


movements, asking the animal to follow commands such as ?person left


Frisbee fetch,? which means ?bring the Frisbee on the left to the person in the


pool?. In one study, two bottlenosed dolphins were tested in proficiency in


interpreting gestural language signs and compared against humans who


viewed the same videos of veridical and degraded gestures. The dolphins


were found to recognize gestures as accurately as fluent humans, and the


results suggested that the dolphins had constructed an interconnected


network of semantic and gestural representations in their memory (Herman,


Morrel-Samuels, & Pack, 1990). Such requests probe the dolphins


understanding of word order and test the animal?s grammatical competence.


It has also been determined that dolphins can form a generalized concept


about an object: they respond correctly to commands involving a hoop, no


matter whether the hoop is round, octagonal, or square. The animals seem to


have a conceptual grasp of the words they learn, showing an understanding of


the core attributes of human language, those being semantics and syntax


(Erickson, 1993). Though this information seems compelling for dolphin


language abilities, to determine whether or not they are capable of complex


intentional communications, researchers must continue to investigate their


receptive capacities, and to attempt to provide them with a communication


system that would tap their productive capacities. Is interspecies


communication possible? Could we someday be having philosophical


discussions with a bottlenosed dolphin? Though these questions seem


ridiculous, there was much debate over these questions when a medical


doctor named John Lilly came out with hopeful findings of dolphin intelligence


in the 1960s (Shane, 1991). In the first true research of dolphin


communication and intelligence, Lilly set out to show that through the


correlation of brain size and IQ, the bottlenose dolphin was perhaps smarter


than humans and began a growing interest in dolphins and their language


through whistles. Though dolphins are exceedingly intelligent creatures, no


real scientific evidence has yet been found to totally support the many


conceptions about the animal?s intelligence. Lilly (1966) states, ?A dolphin . .


. naturally uses other sounds to convey and receive ?meaning?: creaking for


night-time and murky-water finding and recognition, putt-putting and whistles


for exchanges with other dolphins, and even air wailing to excite human


responses in the way of fish or applause. If a dolphin is copying our speech,


he?ll copy that part of what he hears which in his ?language? conveys


meanings.? Although this excerpt shows an incredible capability for dolphins


to produce intelligent communication, it is findings such as these, which lack


scientific support and have lost credibility among other dolphin researchers in


the past few decades. Though his findings lack support, Lilly was important in


bringing forth interest among people and therefore funds towards more


scientifically based research and experiments that have helped us learn more


about communication skills and intelligence of dolphins (Tyack et al. 1989).


In order to clearly understand if dolphins are creating intentional, intelligent


communicative sounds and meanings, it is necessary to break down the vocal


signals into repertoires and analyze those individually. The breaking down of


dolphin signaling into component units has just now begun and the task will be


to discover if, when, and to what extent they structure formalized sequences


of signal units. To determine whether they have a repertoire of grammatical


rules that generates organized sequences will be difficult, and it will be


necessary to obtain extended and continuous recordings. Patterns must be


found and compared to other dolphin recordings in order to obtain the most


accurate and universal findings for language among bottlenose dolphins


(Herman, Kuczjac II, & Holder, 1993). Through many more years of careful


study of these sounds, it is hopeful that our scientists can determine capacities


and meanings behind dolphin language. Though interspecies communication


seems unlikely at this point in time, through new studies being conducted our


conception of dolphins as communicative animals seems more possible.


Intentional communication through gestural understanding is the best finding


so far in the study of these intelligent animals, and leads many to believe there


is a lot more to dolphin?s communication skills than has yet been uncovered.


In tests done in mimicry and labeling of objects, it seems that the capacity the


bottlenose dolphin has for learning and understanding is large enough to make


taught communication a realistic goal in the future of dolphin training. The


highly specialized auditory and vocal mechanisms of the animal have helped


lead the way to a better understanding of cetacean ear anatomy and sound


production mechanisms, and these functions can now be seen as complex


structures unlike any found above water. Though more research needs to be


done before any true conclusions can be made about dolphin language, from


what we do know the bottlenose dolphin is among the most vocal of


nonhuman animals and exhibits remarkable development of sound production


and auditory mechanisms (Schusterman et al. 1986).


Brecht, M. (1993). Communications: A Predictive Theory of


Dolphin Communication. Kybernetes, 22, 39-53. Erickson, D. (1993,


March). Can Animals Think? Time, 146, 182-189. Herman, L. M., Kuczaj


II, S. A., & Holder, M. D. (1993). Responses to Anomalous Gestural


Sequences by a Language-Trained Dolphin: Evidence for Processing of


Semantic Relations and Syntactic Information. Journal of Experimental


Psychology, 122, 184-194. Herman, L. M., Morrel-Samuels, P., & Pack,


A. (1990). Bottlenosed Dolphin and Human Recognition of Veridical and


Degraded Video Displays of an Artificial Gestural Language. Journal of


Experimental Psychology, 119, 215-230. Lilly, J. C., (1966). Lilly on


Dolphins. Garden City, N.Y.: Anchor Books. Anchor Press/Doubleday.


McCowan, B., Hanser, S. F., & Doyle, L.R. (1999). Quantitative tools for


comparing animal communication systems: information theory applied to


bottlenose dolphin whistle repertoires. Animal Behaviour, 57, 409-419.


Popov, V. V., & Supin, A. Y. (1991). Interaural intensity and latency


difference in the dolphin?s auditory system. Neuroscience Letters, 133,


295-297. Schusterman, R. J., Thomas, J. A., & Wood, F. G. (1986).


Dolphin Cognition and Behavior: A Comparitive Approach. London:


Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Shane, S. H. (1991). Smarts.


Seafrontiers, 37, 40-43. Supin, A. Y., Popov, V. V., & Klishin, V. O.


(1993). ABR Frequency Tuning Curves in Dolphins. Journal of Comparitive


Psychology A, 173, 649-656. Tyack, P. L.,& Sayigh, L. S. (1989). These


Dolphins Aren?t Just Whistling in the Dark. Oceanus, 32, 80-83.

Сохранить в соц. сетях:
Обсуждение:
comments powered by Disqus

Название реферата: Dolphins Essay Research Paper Bottlenose dolphins are

Слов:2609
Символов:18598
Размер:36.32 Кб.